Martial arts increase oxytocin production

Yuri Rassovsky, Anna Harwood, Orna Zagoory-Sharon & Ruth Feldman

Scientific reports. Nature. – 2019

Numerous studies have demonstrated that oxytocin (OT), a peptide hormone, plays an important role in regulating mammalian social behaviors, linking it to social affiliation in parent-infant attachment,
romantic and filial relationships, and other prosocial behaviors, such as trust and cooperation. Not surprisingly, research efforts have been made to increase endogenous levels of OT. In the present study,
we investigated whether traditional martial arts training, which integrates the natural benefits of physical exercise with dyadic prosocial interaction, would result in OT response. To this end, 68 beginner
and advanced participants were recruited from several schools practicing Jujitsu (“soft art”), a form of traditional martial arts originating in Japan. Salivary OT levels were assessed at baseline, immediately
following high-intensity training, and following a cool-down period. Analyses revealed a significant increase in OT immediately after a high-intensity training, returning to baseline levels following a cooldown
period. Additionally, although no significant difference between beginner and advanced martial artists was found, a significantly higher increase in salivary OT followed ground grappling, as compared
to “punch-kick” sparring, indicating an added benefit of close contact tactile interaction. These results suggest that the reportedly socially beneficial effects of traditional martial arts may be in part mediated
by OT release and underscore the potentially therapeutic applications of these methods for disorders involving social dysfunction, such as autism, conduct problems, or schizophrenia.

Oxytocin (OT) is a peptide hormone that plays an important role in regulating mammalian social behaviors1. In animals, OT has been shown to support the formation of attachment bonds2. Studies have shown, for example,
that OT mediated maternal behaviors, such as licking and grooming in rats3, olfactory recognition of offspring in sheep4, and the grooming and contact of Rhesus Macaques5. These effects are paralleled in humans, linking OT to
social affiliation in parent-child attachment6,7. For instance, synchronous interactions that involve physical touch between parents and young children were shown to increase endogenous OT production in both healthy infants6
and preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders8. Subsequent studies also showed OT release in romantic and filial relationships9,10, as well as other prosocial behaviors, such as trust and cooperation11.

These potentially beneficial effects of OT have naturally resulted in efforts to increase endogenous levels of OT. Although several studies have reported therapeutic effects of oral, intravenous, and intranasal administration
of OT in disorders of social dysfunction, such as autism and schizophrenia12–14, substantial challenges remain regarding the passage of OT through the blood-brain barrier15. As such, there is a continued need to
explore non-pharmacological approaches for increasing endogenous OT. A potentially promissing way to naturally increase OT levels is through physical exercise. Indeed, several studies have suggested that exercise-induced
increases in OT may be important for modulating cardiovascular changes and fluid homeostasis during and following exercise and may also moderate stress-induced response. For example, an early study in rats has shown
that OT is released in the complex involving the nucleus of the solitary tract and the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus to restrain exercise-induced tachycardia16. A more recent study reported that forced swimming in
rats induced OT release into the blood plasma and the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus17. A few studies in humans suggested similar results. Two small trials in healthy participants reported an increase in OT following a
prolonged running exercise18,19, and a recent study showed salivary OT concentrations increases following moderate 10-minute running, remaining significantly above baseline 40 minutes after completion of the exercise20.

The beneficial effects of physical exercise on physical, cognitive, and emotional well-being are well documented in healthy individuals21, as well as many medical and psychiatric illnesses22–24. This ever-increasing perception of physical exercise as medicine is nicely illustrated in the citation from an interview with Dr. Robert Sallis, the president of the American College of Sports Medicine, stating that “if we had a pill that conferred all the proven health benefits of exercise, physicians would widely prescribe it to their patients and our healthcare system would see to it that every patient had access to this wonder drug”25. One type of sport that confers the
benefit of physical exercise and involves dyadic prosocial interaction is traditional martial arts. Over the past half century, martial arts have gained increasing popularity in the West, as their positive effects on cognitive functions,
self-regulation, and sense of well-being have been demonstrated26,27. The philosophy underpinning traditional martial arts is one of attaining the Zen state of mushin (“no mindedness”). This describes a state whereby the
participant is capable of “fighting” to their fullest extent but without aggressive feelings. Such balance is achieved through ritualization of combat moves and the requirement of respect to the instructor, practice space, and one
another, as well as by highlighting the importance of meditation and philosophies such as peace, benevolence, humanity, and self-restraint28.

Research into the martial arts has focused on those elements that are most valuable to the targeted population. Research with adolescents and young adults look at the benefits of martial arts in teaching self-control,
enhancing self-esteem, teaching a more positive response to physical challenges, and inducing greater emotional stability, self-confidence, and assertiveness. Martial arts provide an outlet for participants to channel energy
into a productive and self-enhancing activity29. They have also been demonstrated to improve concentration and self-awareness in children30 and enhance executive functions31, including self-monitoring, awareness32 and
cognitive-regulation30.

In the present study, we examined the effects of martial arts training on OT response. To this end, beginner and advanced participants were recruited from several schools practicing Jujitsu (“soft art”): Dennis Survival
Jujitsu (DSJJ) and Brazilian Jujitsu (BJJ). Both approaches have originated from Japanese martial art and integrate the aforementioned aspects of traditional martial arts into their practice. Additionally, both forms include a randori
component (high-intensity, free-style friendly tournament) in each class. However, whereas randori in DSJJ typically involves “punch-kick” sparring, BJJ focuses on ground grappling. Thus, we sought to address the following
three questions. First, given the early suggestions connecting physical exercise and OT, we examined whether the high-intensity aerobic training during martial arts would result in exercise-induced increases in OT. Second,
as beginner and advanced participants have had substantially different levels of prior martial arts training, we investigated whether this might lead to differential OT responses. Finally, we examined whether the longer close
contact time occurring during ground grappling would result in greater OT response.

Results
To examine the OT response between beginner and advanced martial artists, LMM was conducted, with trainee level (beginner vs. advanced), time of saliva collection (baseline, peak-training, cool-down), and their interactions
as fixed factors, and an intercept for subject as a random factor. These analyses demonstrated a significant effect of time of saliva collection, F (2, 120) = 12.0, p < 0.001, with post-hoc comparisons indicating a significant
change from baseline to peak-training (pbonferroni < 0.001), as well as from peak-training to post-cooldown, (pbonferroni < 0.001). However, neither the main effect of belt level, F (1, 60) = 0.79, p = 0.38, nor the interaction between
time of saliva collection and belt level, F (2, 120) = 0.10, p = 0.91, were statistically significant (see Fig. 1).
LMM was then employed to examine potential differences in OT response between the different types of training across time, such that type of randori (sparring vs. grappling), time of saliva collection (baseline,
peak-training, cool-down), and their interactions were entered as fixed factors and an intercept for subject as a random factor. These analyses revealed a significant effect of time of saliva collection, F (2, 132) = 15.6,
p < 0.001, with post-hoc comparisons again indicating a significant change from baseline to peak-training (pbonferroni < 0.001), as well as from peak-training to post-cooldown, (pbonferroni < 0.001). Furthermore, whereas the main
effect of type of randori did not reach statistical significance, F (1, 66) = 3.47, p = 0.07, the interaction between time of saliva collection and type of randori was significant, F (2, 132) = 4.69, p = 0.01, (see Fig. 2). As can be seen

in Fig. 2, further exploration of the interaction through post-hoc comparisons of the simple main effects indicated that a significant difference in OT response between grappling (M = 53.6, SE = 6.28) and sparring (M = 38.1,SE = 2.39) occurred only immediately following peak-training (pbonferroni = 0.04). This difference was further confirmed by comparing the areas under the curve, such that the AUCG in the grappling condition (M = 2409,
SE = 677) was significantly larger than the AUCG in the sparring condition (M = 731, SE = 147.5, p = 0.006).
Similarly, the AUCI in the grappling condition (M = 940, SE = 371) was significantly larger than the AUCI in the sparring condition (M = 124, SE = 75.6, p = 0.01).
Discussion
The current study was a pioneering effort to examine whether a session of traditional martial arts training would  induce an increase in OT levels. We found a significant increase in OT immediately after a high-intensity randori
session, returning to baseline levels following a cool-down period. Additionally, although no significant difference between beginner and advanced martial artists was found, a significant interaction between time of
saliva collection and type of randori indicated that grappling resulted in a significantly higher OT increase than sparring. Given the beneficial effects of traditional martial arts on cognitive and psychological functions30,31, and
the reported therapeutic effects of exogenous administration of OT in disorders of social dysfunction, such as autism and schizophrenia12–14, this readily available, non-invasive training program may have wide implications
for physical fitness and community health.
Several potential mechanisms may underlie OT release during randori. As mentioned above, a few animal and human studies have reported OT increase following aerobic exercise17,20. Thus, it is possible that the high-intensity
physical activity occurring during randori may, in itself, lead to increases in OT. However, another more complex social mechanism that may contribute to OT increase during randori is the ability of OT to increase the salience
of social information33. Early studies in sheep, for example, reported that OT promotes the selective olfactory recognition of offspring4. In humans, administered OT was found to increase gaze toward eye regions, which are
considered the most socially communicative part of faces34. Moreover, several neuroimaging studies have shown that the administration of OT can have effects on socially-relevant brain areas including the amygdala35 and the
ventral tegmental area33, and studies using fMRI36 found that the tendency toward increasing gaze to the eyes is associated with an increased coupling of amygdala and superior colliculi activity, supporting the view that OT
biases an individual toward social visual information.
These key features of increased social information salience are crucial in the interpersonal synchrony, which has been associated with OT function. The OT molecule was shown to be critically involved with repetitive synchronous
behaviors in a range of living organisms, from coordination of reproductive behavior in roundworms to flocking behavior in birds37. Furthermore, in humans, it has been demonstrated that increased levels of OT are
related to increased synchronous gaze between mothers and their children38 and neural synchrony during social coordination39. Thus, the emphasis on dyadic synchronous behaviors in martial arts may contribute to increases
in salivary OT. Of course, additional studies are needed to examine whether this synchronous behavior common in martial arts results in greater OT release, as compared to other, solitary physical activities described above20.
It should be noted that although the presumably OT-mediated bias toward social information has commonly been associated with prosocial behaviors, several studies have reported OT-related antisocial behaviors, especially
in the context of competitive situations40. For instance, increases in OT have been associated with increased parochial altruism, or the drive towards “tend and defend”41. This refers to the promotion of prosocial behaviors
and empathetic responses to the in-group, while increasing defensive actions, although not proactive aggression, towards the out-group. This phenomenon has been demonstrated in hypothetically threatening41 and unpredictably
startling42 laboratory situations. Additionally, antisocial behaviors following increased OT have been observed in threatening situations, such as protecting the young, mating, and out-group conflict40. Interestingly,
in a recent study comparing a combat-trained and control non-combat male veterans, OT administration was found to attenuate the response to combat stimuli and increased responses to pleasant social scenes, whereas for
non-combat controls OT administration increased social salience when presented with combat-related cues43.
Thus, it appears that both prosocial and antisocial, OT-mediated, responses may be strongly related to contextual and individually predisposing factors, making their universal applicability limited11.

These studies underscore the complex relationship between OT and social behavior and may offer a potential underpinning for connecting increases in OT release during the randori component in martial arts, as this
high-intensity, free-style dyadic interaction, involving a simultaneous attack at and defense against one’s opponent, clearly requires very high levels of social visual information processing and social interaction. Traditional
martial arts practice involves an intensive dyadic effort during a round of sparring, while capitalizing on interpersonal synchrony and social group cohesion. This combination of pro-social synchronous behaviors with competitive
sparring may both contribute to increased OT responsivity during a martial arts session.
The significantly greater post-grappling OT response, as compared to sparring, may also be informative. A recent study investigated OT release and associated neural responses during hand- or machine-administered
massage in 40 adult male subjects44. The authors reported that hand-administered massage increased plasma OT release and activity in brain regions involved in social cognition and reward. Additionally, whereas previous
research has demonstrated no difference between lone running and lone sexual stimulation in terms of salivary OT increase20, the present findings suggest that the intensive interpersonal contact may be especially conducive
to OT response. Grappling randori naturally involves a continuous, intensive tactile interaction between the trainees,
and, despite the competitive nature of the interaction, requires a significant degree of prosocial cooperation and support throughout the training process. Furthermore, the greater OT response in grappling as opposed to
the sparring group, irrespective of expertise, suggests that this response is likely not related to greater anxiety or stress, which would be expected to be greater in the novice group (although this assumption should be directly
evaluated in future research).
Mutually agreeable touch, in general, as opposed to non-touch relaxation methods, has been demonstrated to reduce anxiety and impart beneficial effects for social development45, with interventions such as massage therapy
reducing anxiety in the elderly46 and aggression in adolescents47. Additionally, it has been shown that greater parental touch is predictive of a greater rise in offspring’s salivary OT following a dyadic interaction48, and preliminary
research has indicated that lower OT levels may be predictive of the level of touch an individual seeks (e.g., the extent to which dog owners pet and touch their canine companions)49. Thus, given that both overt
relaxation-intended touch (e.g., massage therapy) and non-focused touch (e.g., dog petting and interpersonal tactile interaction) lead to increased OT, it is not surprising that the extensive tactile interaction involved in grappling
may lead to enhanced OT response.
Several limitations of the present study, as well as potential venues for future research, should be acknowledged.
First, as suggested above, there could be several explanations for the increase in OT levels during training, such as an exercise-induced OT response and/or a more complex mediation by increasing the salience of social
information. Future studies may be able to disentangle these potential mechanisms by comparing, for example, aerobic activities that require attention to socially-relevant information for optimal performance with those that
do not require social interactions. Second, due to the “rough” nature of the dyadic interaction during randori, it was not possible to monitor physiological parameters of physical exercises. However, as indicated above, simulations
of non-contact high-intensity randori indicated that participants in both groups maintained heart rate zones comparable with high-intensity interval training. Therefore, the significantly higher OT response during grappling,
as compared to sparring, cannot be explained simply by differences in training intensity, but rather likely occurred due to the larger tactile stimulation occurring during the former type of training. Nonetheless, it would
be informative to adapt physiological measure of exercise intensity levels to be used during martial arts training, in order to examine potential relationships of these parameters with increases in OT.
Additionally, the current study did not include any emotional or behavioral measures that may be related to OT response. Indeed, a recent study employed exercise-induced OT stimulation in patients with craniopharyngioma
(epithelial tumors thought to impair OT production and release), to examine whether altered oxytocin levels would account for affective and emotional dysfunction50. The authors reported a positive association between
baseline OT and trait anxiety and a negative association between OT response to exercise and state anxiety. This study underscores the complexity of the association among exercise, OT, and emotional functioning and paves
the way for future studies, requiring multilevel assessments (e.g., physiological, behavioral, and functional) in order to more fully understand this complex relationship.
It should also be noted that the use of saliva to assess OT has not been without criticism since its early application in neuroendocrinological research. For example, some researchers suggested non-specific binding to
non-OT antibodies51 and others raised the issue of questionable correlation with plasma OT52. In recent years, however, there have been substantial improvements in OT testing kits38,53, reducing potential non-specific binding.
Research has furthermore demonstrated associations between salivary OT and brain activation in areas rich in OT receptors54, as well as increases in salivary OT following intranasal administration55. Importantly, consistent
with the current study, recent studies have demonstrated salivary OT response to physical activities in primates56 and humans50. Thus, despite reasonable early criticisms, salivary OT has become in recent years a
well-researched and accepted measure across numerous labs around the world.
Finally, the present cross-sectional study was designed to examine the immediate OT response to training, rather than tracking changes in OT release over time. Although the inclusion of beginner and advanced trainees
bear some relevance to this issue, the large individual differences inherent in OT production may have prevented meaningful comparisons. Thus, prospective studies following martial arts trainees over time are needed to examine
whether acquisition of expertise in this field may lead to differential OT responses.
In sum, the present study is the first to examine the effects of martial arts training on OT response. Findings demonstrated that both types of training, but especially the component involving intense tactile interaction,
resulted in significant release of salivary OT. Traditional martial arts typically involve a dyadic, stimulating, and prosocial physical activity and have gained increasing popularity in the last several decades, due to demonstrated
positive effects on cognitive functions, self-regulation, and sense of well-being26,27. Given the underlying philosophy of traditional martial arts, emphasizing during training the importance of benevolence, humanity, andself-restraint28, it is not surprising that OT release is a potential mechanism underlying its beneficial effects. This stimulating physical activity can be readily integrated into diverse community and clinical settings, and, with
additional research and adaptation to clinical populations, may potentially offer therapeutic applications in disorders of social dysfunction.
Methods
Participants. Participants were recruited from several martial arts schools across the central region of Israel. A total of 68 healthy males (mean age = 27.1, SD = 12.6) participated in the study, of which 30 were advanced martial artists (black belts) and the rest beginners (white or yellow belts). Forty participants practiced DSJJ (21 black belts) and 28 practiced BJJ (9 black belts). Participants were excluded if they had an identifiable medical or
psychiatric condition or taking current or regular medications. They were asked to refrain from alcohol, caffeine, or nicotine consumption on the day of the experiment. All methods were performed in accordance with the
relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants gave written informed consent after receiving a full explanation of the research according to procedures approved by the Institutional Review Board at Bar-Ilan University,
the Israel Ministry of Education Ethics committee, and the Helsinki Ethics committee of Hadassah Hospital in Jerusalem, as required by the Ministry of Health. For participants under 18 years of age, written informed consent
was also obtained from their parents.
Saliva sampling and oxytocin measurement. Salivary OT measures have been validated across various studies and become a common practice in recent years. For example, salivary OT measurements have been shown
to correlate with plasma OT57 and have demonstrated consistency across the life-course58. Additionally, increasing OT artificially was shown to parallel increases in salivary OT55. Thus, given that the present study focused on
repeated measures of OT over a short duration, salivary OT was deemed the most efficient and effective method of assessment.
Three saliva samples were collected at baseline, immediately after the peak training intensity, and following a cool-down period. Participants gave saliva by passive drooling into a clean 5 ml tube. If the participant had difficulty
producing sufficient saliva, they were instructed to massage their jaw and imagine food on their tongue.
Participants were told that they may drink water immediately following saliva production but refrain from drinking until following the next saliva sampling. All samples were then stored at −20 °C.
In order to precipitate the mucus, samples underwent three freeze-thaw cycles: freeze at −80 °C and thaw at 4 °C. After the fourth cycle the tubes were centrifuged twice at 1500 g (4000 rpm) for 30 minutes each. Supernatant
was collected and the aliquots stored at −20 °C until assayed. In order to increase the sensitivity, the liquid samples were concentrated four times by freeze-drying for 3–4 days to yield a powder. Prior the freeze-drying
procedure the samples were stored at −80 °C, for at least three days. OT concentration was measured using a commercial OT Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay (ELISA) kit (Cayman Chemicals, Ann Arbor, Michigan,
USA). The kit provides quantitative in vitro assay for OT in human saliva. The dry samples were reconstructed with the assay buffer immediately before the assay. Measurement was done in duplicate, according to the kit’s
instructions. The concentration of OT in the samples were calculated using MatLab (The Natworks, Natick, MA) according to relevant standard curves. The intra-assay coefficients of samples and controls were less than 12.4%
and 14.5%, respectively.
Procedure. To maximize attendance, data collection was scheduled for a specific date, and participants were provided with a summary of the research by their instructors during the previous week. All data collection took
place during the summer months in the evening after dark, to control for seasonal/daily hormonal fluctuations.
After the researchers explained the study procedures and obtained written informed consent (minors provided parental informed consent prior to the scheduled date), demographic information (i.e., age, fitness to participate,
martial arts trainee level) was collected. Participants then provided a baseline saliva sample and began the martial arts training session. Through prior coordination between the researchers and martial arts instructors,
the sessions were standardized across schools to include the following components: an approximate 10-minute warm-up, 15-minute technique practice, 20-minutes of high-intensity randori, and a 15-minute cool-down
period (see Fig. 3). Thus, the training sessions were generally similar, except for the randori component, such that DSJJ trainees engaged in a typical “punch-kick” sparring, whereas BJJ engaged in ground grappling. Giventhe rough interactions during the randori component, it was not possible to monitor physiological parameters of physical exercises using standard heart rate (HR) monitors, such as arm bands or chest straps. However, simulation
of non-contact high-intensity randori indicated that participants in both groups maintained 1–3 min periods at 85%-95% of individual HRmax, alternating with 30–60 sec periods at 60%-70% of individual HRmax (estimated
using standard HR zone calculation). This form of physical exercise would be considered high-intensity interval training (HIIT), interspersed with active recovery periods59,60. The second saliva sample was obtained immediately
following the high-intensity randori, and the third sample was taken following the cool-down period. At the end of the session, researchers provided additional information about the nature of the study and answered any
questions from the participants.
Data analysis. To test for differences in OT response, we fitted the data with a Linear Mixed Model (LMM), using R’s lme4 package61. The advantage of mixed effects models is that they account for variability between subjects
and correlations within the data and are superior to Repeated Measures ANOVA in handling missing data62. The fixed factors of the fitted models included type of randori (sparring vs. grappling), trainee level (beginner vs.
advanced), and time of saliva collection (baseline, peak-training, cool-down), as well as their interactions. A random intercept for subject was also included in the model. Significant results were followed by post-hoc analyses
with Bonferroni correction. We also employed standard formulas63 to compute the areas under the curve with respect to ground (AUCG) and with respect to increase (AUCI) and used t-tests to compare the total OT production
between the groups. Because several participants did not produce sufficient saliva for analysis at different time points (four participants at baseline, seven following peak training, and twelve following cool-down), missing
data was handled by repeating the analyses using maximum-likelihood expectation-maximization64, which yielded virtually identical results.

Data Availability
Data are available upon request.
References
1. Carter, C. S., Williams, J. R., Witt, D. M. & Insel, T. R. Oxytocin and social bonding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 652,
204–211 (1992).
2. Insel, T. R. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron 65,
768–779 (2010).
3. Francis, D. D., Young, L. J., Meaney, M. J. & Insel, T. R. Naturally occurring differences in maternal care are associated with the
expression of oxytocin and vasopressin (V1a) receptors: Gender differences. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 14, 349–353 (2002).
4. Kendrick, K. M. et al. Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Research Bulletin 44,
383–395 (1997).
5. Maestripieri, D., Hoffman, C. L., Anderson, G. M., Carter, C. S. & Higley, J. D. Mother-infant interactions in free-ranging rhesus
macaques: Relationships between physiological and behavioral variables. Physiology & Behavior 96, 613–619 (2009).
6. Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Schneiderman, I., Weisman, O. & Zagoory-Sharon, O. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are
associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1133–1141 (2010).
7. Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F. & Feldman, R. Oxytocin and the Development of Parenting in Humans. Biological
Psychiatry 68, 377–382 (2010).
8. Feldman, R., Golan, O., Hirschler-Guttenberg, Y., Ostfeld-Etzion, S. & Zagoory-Sharon, O. Parent child interaction and oxytocin
production in pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 205, 107–112 (2014).
9. Schneiderman, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F. & Feldman, R. Oxytocin during the initial stages of romantic attachment:
Relations to couples’ interactive reciprocity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1277–1285 (2012).
10. Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Influs, M., Gutbir, T. & Ebstein, R. P. Parental Oxytocin and Early Caregiving Jointly Shape Children’s
Oxytocin Response and Social Reciprocity. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1154–1162 (2013).
11. Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N. & Ochsner, K. N. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends in cognitive
sciences 15, 301–309 (2011).
12. Davis, M. C. et al. Oxytocin-Augmented Social Cognitive Skills Training in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 39,
2070–2077 (2014).
13. Guastella, A. J. & Hickie, I. B. Oxytocin Treatment, Circuitry, and Autism: A Critical Review of the Literature Placing Oxytocin Into
the Autism Context. Biological Psychiatry 79, 234–242 (2016).
14. Parker, K. J. et al. Intranasal oxytocin treatment for social deficits and biomarkers of response in children with autism. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114, 8119–8124 (2017).
15. Chini, B., Leonzino, M., Braida, D. & Sala, M. Learning About Oxytocin: Pharmacologic and Behavioral Issues. Biological psychiatry
(2013).
16. Braga, D. C., Mori, E., Higa, K. T., Morris, M. & Michelini, L. C. Central oxytocin modulates exercise-induced tachycardia. American
Journal of Physiology-Regulatory Integrative and Comparative Physiology 278, R1474–R1482 (2000).
17. Torner, L., Plotsky, P. M., Neumann, I. D. & de Jong, T. R. Forced swimming-induced oxytocin release into blood and brain: Effects
of adrenalectomy and corticosterone treatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 77, 165–174 (2017).
18. Landgraf, R., Hacker, R. & Buhl, H. Plasma Vasopressin and Oxytocin in Response to Exercise and During A Day-Night Cycle in
Man. Endokrinologie 79, 281–291 (1982).
19. Hew-Butler, T., Noakes, T. D., Soldin, S. J. & Verbalis, J. G. Acute changes in endocrine and fluid balance markers during highintensity,
steady-state, and prolonged endurance running: unexpected increases in oxytocin and brain natriuretic peptide during
exercise. European Journal of Endocrinology 159, 729–737 (2008).
20. de Jong, T. R. et al. Salivary oxytocin concentrations in response to running, sexual self-stimulation, breastfeeding and the TSST: The
Regensburg Oxytocin Challenge (ROC) study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62, 381–388 (2015).
21. Cotman, C. W. & Berchtold, N. C. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences
25, 295–301 (2002).
22. Rosenbaum, S., Tiedemann, A., Sherrington, C., Curtis, J. & Ward, P. B. Physical Activity Interventions for People With Mental
Illness: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 75, 964–U171 (2014).
23. Wisloff, U. et al. Superior cardiovascular effect of aerobic interval training versus moderate continuous training in heart failure
patients – A randomized study. Circulation 115, 3086–3094 (2007).
24. Malchow, B. et al. The effects of physical exercise in schizophrenia and affective disorders. European Archives of Psychiatry and
Clinical Neuroscience 263, 451–467 (2013).
25. Berryman, J. W. Exercise is Medicine: A Historical Perspective. Current Sports Medicine Reports 9, 195–201 (2010).26. Vertonghen, J. & Theeboom, M. The social-psychological outcomes of martial arts practise among youth: A review. Journal of Sports
Science and Medicine 9, 528–537 (2010).
27. Harwood, A., Lavidor, M. & Rassovsky, Y. Reducing aggression with martial arts: A meta-analysis of child and youth studies.
Aggression and Violent Behavior 34, 96–101 (2017).
28. Nosanchuk, T. A. & Macneil, M. L. C. Examination Of The Effects Of Traditional and Modern Martial Arts Training On
Aggressiveness. Aggressive Behavior 15, 153–159 (1989).
29. Twemlow, S. W. & Sacco, F. C. The application of traditional martial arts practice and theory to the treatment of violent adolescents.
Adolescence 33, 505–518 (1998).
30. Lakes, K. D. & Hoyt, W. I. Promoting self-regulation through school-based martial arts training. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology 25, 283–302 (2004).
31. Diamond, A. & Lee, K. Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old. Science 333,
959–964 (2011).
32. Lothes, J., Hakan, R. & Kasab, K. Aikido Experience And Its Relation To Mindfulnes: A Two-Part Study. Perceptual and Motor Skills
116, 30–39 (2013).
33. Groppe, S. E. et al. Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain.
Biological psychiatry 74, 172–179 (2013).
34. Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B. & Dadds, M. R. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biological psychiatry 63, 3–5
(2008).
35. Kirsch, P. et al. Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. The Journal of neuroscience: the official
journal of the Society for Neuroscience 25, 11489–11493 (2005).
36. Gamer, M., Zurowski, B. & Buchel, C. Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in
humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 9400–9405 (2010).
37. Feldman, R. The neurobiology of mammalian parenting and the biosocial context of human caregiving. Hormones and Behavior 77,
3–17 (2016).
38. Weisman, O., Zagoory-Sharon, O. & Feldman, R. Oxytocin Administration to Parent Enhances Infant Physiological and Behavioral
Readiness for Social Engagement. Biological Psychiatry 72, 982–989 (2012).
39. Mu, Y., Guo, C. Y. & Han, S. H. Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience 11, 1882–1893 (2016).
40. Beery, A. K. Antisocial oxytocin: complex effects on social behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 6, 174–182 (2015).
41. De Dreu, C. K. W. et al. The Neuropeptide Oxytocin Regulates Parochial Altruism in Intergroup Conflict Among Humans. Science
328, 1408–1411 (2010).
42. Grillon, C. et al. Oxytocin increases anxiety to unpredictable threat. Molecular Psychiatry 18, 958–960 (2013).
43. Levy, J. et al. Oxytocin selectively modulates brain response to stimuli probing social synchrony. Neuroimage 124, 923–930 (2016).
44. Li, Q. et al. Foot massage evokes oxytocin release and activation of orbitofrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 101, 193–203 (2019).
45. Cascio, C. J., Moore, D. & McGlone, F. Social touch and human development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 35, 5–11 (2019).
46. Yücel, Ş. Ç., Arslan, G. G. & Bagci, H. Effects of hand massage and therapeutic touch on comfort and anxiety living in a nursing
home in turkey: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Religion and Health, No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified
(2019).
47. Diego, M. A. et al. Aggressive adolescents benefit from massage therapy. Adolescence 37, 597–607 (2002).
48. Lebowitz, E. R. et al. Oxytocin response to youth-mother interactions in clinically anxious youth is associated with separation
anxiety and dyadic behavior. Depression and Anxiety 34, 127–136 (2017).
49. Petersson, M. et al. Oxytocin and Cortisol Levels in Dog Owners and Their Dogs Are Associated with Behavioral Patterns: An
Exploratory Study. Frontiers in Psychology 8 (2017).
50. Gebert, D. et al. De-masking oxytocin-deficiency in craniopharyngioma and assessing its link with affective function.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 88, 61–69 (2018).
51. McCullough, M. E., Churchland, P. S. & Mendez, A. J. Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: Can the data on oxytocin and
human behavior be trusted? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37, 1485–1492 (2013).
52. Quintana, D. S. et al. Saliva oxytocin measures do not reflect peripheral plasma concentrations after intranasal oxytocin
administration in men. Hormones and Behavior 102, 85–92 (2018).
53. Daughters, K. et al. Salivary Oxytocin Concentrations in Males following Intranasal Administration of Oxytocin: A Double-Blind,
Cross-Over Study. PloS one 10 (2015).
54. Abraham, E. et al. The Human Coparental Bond Implicates Distinct Corticostriatal Pathways: Longitudinal Impact on Family
Formation and Child Well-Being. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2301–2313 (2017).
55. Weisman, O., Zagoory-Sharon, O. & Feldman, R. Intranasal oxytocin administration is reflected in human saliva.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1582–1586 (2012).
56. Leeds, A. et al. Validating the use of a commercial enzyme immunoassay to measure oxytocin in unextracted urine and saliva of the
western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates 59, 499–515 (2018).
57. Feldman, R., Gordon, I. & Zagoory-Sharon, O. Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent-infant
synchrony: considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Developmental Science 14, 752–761 (2011).
58. Priel, A., Djalovski, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O. & Feldman, R. Maternal depression impacts child psychopathology across the first
decade of life: Oxytocin and synchrony as markers of resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 60, 30–42 (2019).
59. Gibala, M. J., Little, J. P., MacDonald, M. J. & Hawley, J. A. Physiological adaptations to low-volume, high-intensity interval training
in health and disease. Journal of Physiology-London 590, 1077–1084 (2012).
60. Lunt, H., Draper, N., Marshall, H. C., Logan, F. J. & Hamlin, M. J. High Intensity Interval Training in a Real World Setting: A
Randomized Controlled Feasibility Study in Overweight Inactive Adults, Measuring Change in Maximal Oxygen Uptake (vol 9,
e83256, 2014). Plos One 9 (2014).
61. Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B. M. & Walker, S. C. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software
67, 1–48 (2015).
62. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J. & Bates, D. M. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of
Memory and Language 59, 390–412 (2008).
63. Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G. & Hellhammer, D. H. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve
represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916–931 (2003).
64. Jamshidian, M. & Bentler, P. M. ML estimation of mean and covariance structures with missing data using complete data routines.
Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics 24, 21–41 (1999).
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Ministry of Science, Technology & Space, Israel (Grant #3-13631) to Drs. Rassovsky and Feldman. We thank the instructors and students for opening their dojos and participating in our research.Author

Contributions
Y.R. conceptualized the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. A.H. conducted the study and assisted with study conceptualization, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. O.Z.S. assisted with data
interpretation and manuscript preparation. R.F. assisted with study conceptualization, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation.

Additional Information
Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted
by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2019

If you like this post, you can write comment or subscribe to RSS and receive every new post of this blog

Comments

Nobody has commented yet

Post your comment

(required)

(required)